Brainier Birds Live On Islands

Kea parrots (Nestor notabilis), endemic to the oceanic island of New Zealand, have a well-deserved reputation for being wickedly intelligent and, well, extremely mischievous.
(Credit: Phillip Capper / CC BY 2.0)

If youโ€™re an evolutionary biologist, thereโ€™s nothing on Earth thatโ€™s quite as special as an island. This is because islands are natural laboratories for studying evolution. Yet, at the same time, evolution on islands is, well โ€ฆ peculiar โ€” peculiar. For example, species that colonize islands show some very predictable evolutionary trends: vertebrates tend towards โ€œmedium body sizesโ€ (this is the so-called โ€œisland ruleโ€; ref), lizards converge towards equivalent eco-morphs (eco-morphs are species that occupy the same niche, have similar morphology and behavior, but are not necessarily close relatives; ref), and birds undergo a reduction or complete loss of flight capacity (ref).

A New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides), and her tool.
(Credit: James St Clair.)

But the propensity for tool-use behaviors, such as those demonstrated by the New Caledonian crow, , the Hawaiian crow, , and the Galaฬpagos woodpecker finch, , may be another peculiarity of island life for birds. Although weโ€™re not yet certain, this behavioral idiosyncrasy appears to stem from island birds evolving advanced cognitive abilities, a trait that may be accompanied by an enlarged brain (ref). What is it about islands that seem to support the evolution of larger brains in birds? Basically, three main features of island life could set the stage for this: niche expansions, such as eating novel foods, environmental variability combined with constraints on dispersal, and a slower pace of life.

First, the paucity of species on islands gives new colonizers the opportunity to expand into novel niches without competition with other species or pressure from their enemies โ€” although they do end up facing increased competition from members of their own species. But it is this intra-specific competition, along with limited opportunities to disperse, that may drive enhanced cognition and the evolution of larger brains (ref). Indeed, there is evidence to support the idea that niche expansion, along with the tendency for behavioral innovation (ref), are tied to evolution of larger brain size.

Second, the relative isolation of islands prevents individuals from fleeing to other places when environmental conditions deteriorate, which forces individuals to develop more elaborate behavioral responses. Despite taking longer to develop and being physiologically costly to maintain, a larger brain may buffer individuals against environmental changes by supporting new or more complex behaviors (ref). For example, in woodpecker finches, tool use replaces the more usual gleaning technique in years when droughts drive insects from foliage to crevices (ref). Thus, environmental variability combined with constraints on dispersal could be a powerful selective force for an enlarged brain in island bird species.

Last but not least, island life is slower than mainland life (ref), and this could facilitate the evolution of a larger brain in island species โ€” and this larger brain takes longer to develop. Since learning and behavioral flexibility ultimately provide big payoffs to island species when local conditions become more challenging, such an evolutionary innovation could quickly spread through the population.

In spite of these credible arguments, the difference in brain size between island and mainland species has only ever been investigated twice. Neither of those studies uncovered any associations, but both suffered from methodological problems, with the most important being that it was unclear whether large brains evolved before or after island colonization. This is especially problematic because we know that larger brains promote exploration and colonization of novel areas (ref), so this creates a โ€œchicken and eggโ€ scenario: which came first, a larger brain or island colonization?

To address the problems faced by the previous two studies, evolutionary ecologist Ferran Sayol, a PhD candidate at the Centre for Research on Ecology and Forestry Applications (CREAF) in Catalonia, Spain, and an international team of collaborators, tested the โ€˜brainโ€“islandโ€™ hypothesis by asking two questions: Is a larger relative brain size a consequence of island living? Which came first, a larger brain or island living?

Is a larger relative brain size a result of island living?

To answer these questions, Mr. Sayol and his collaborators assembled brain measurements for 11,554 museum specimens (110 species living on islands that rise from the ocean floor due to seismic or volcanic activity, and 1,821 continental species) and phylogenetically analyzed them. These birds represented more than 1900 species that belonged to 91% of all living bird families. Mr. Sayol and his collaborators classified those species as either inhabiting oceanic islands (110) or not (1821) and statistically analyzed whether island species have larger brains than mainland species. They found that endemic oceanic island birds do have bigger brains than mainland-dwelling birds (Figure 1), even after controlling for migratory behavior.

Fig. 1 The effect of insularity on relative brain size. a Oceanic island birds have relatively bigger brains than other birds (boxplots represent median and percentiles [2.5, 25, 75 and 97.5 %]). b From the posterior samples of the BMPP models, we can see a consistent effect of insularity on relative brain size across phylogenies coming from two different backbones from the global avian phylogeny.
(doi:10.1038/s41467โ€“018โ€“05280โ€“8)

Which came first, a larger brain or island living?

Although it is true that alien species introduced by humans were more likely to become established in their novel homes, including islands, if they had a larger brain to begin with (i.e.; ref), there are two more pieces to this โ€˜brainโ€“islandโ€™ hypothesis puzzle that suggested that the enlarged brains of island bird species primarily evolved island colonization rather than before. First, island-dwelling bird species were more likely to be from brainier avian families, since common colonizers of oceanic islands include both small-brained lineages, such as pigeons and rails, and large-brained lineages, such as crows and parrots. Second, when Mr. Sayol and his collaborators reconstructed bird family relationships and mapped island species onto that phylogeny, they saw no difference in relative brain size between the ancestors of oceanic island species and the ancestors of their continental-living cousins (Figure 2a).

Fig. 2 Reconstructions of island colonizations and relative brain size changes. a The phylogenetic distribution of oceanic island-living birds (110 species coloured in red)*. Internal branches show estimated transitions from island to mainland and vice versa in one of the ancestral estimations, where red and blue branches represent island and mainland living, respectively.
(doi:10.1038/s41467โ€“018โ€“05280โ€“8)

Yet, at the same time, the descendants of island species had relatively larger brains than their mainland ancestors, suggesting that evolution towards relatively larger brains occurred island colonization (Figure 2b & c)

Fig. 2 Reconstructions of island colonizations and relative brain size changes. b Representation of ancestors and descendants in the different types of transitions studied: continent to continent (ฮฑ), continent to islands (ฮฒ), and islands to islands (ฮณ). c Histograms show the difference in relative brain size (RB) between different ancestral types, comparing the posterior samples of each category from the BPMM. Bird silhouettes were drawn by FS and are available at PhyloPic.
(doi:10.1038/s41467โ€“018โ€“05280โ€“8)

Alternative explanations of the observations

Mr. Sayol and his collaborators examined which of three main mechanisms at play on islands might explain why oceanic islands bring about increases in relative brain size in birds: niche expansions, differences in life history, and environmental variation with limited dispersal opportunities (Figure 3a).

Fig. 3 Alternative mechanisms behind changes in relative brain size in islands. a Different factors could be mediating the evolution of larger relative brains on islands. Niche expansions (1), changes in life history (2) and increased environmental variation (3). b The best-supported path model (lowest CICc) suggests that life history and environmental factors are mainly responsible for brain expansions. Abbreviations: INS insularity, DBR diet breadth, DVP developmental period, IVE inter-annual variation in environmental productivity, RBS relative brain size. P > 0.05 indicates that the model fits the data. Silhouettes were drawn by FS.
(doi:10.1038/s41467โ€“018โ€“05280โ€“8)

Mr. Sayol and his collaborators fitted a model that allowed the three mechanisms to compete to explain their data. (This is path analysis.) They found that birds living on oceanic islands tend to have larger brains than closely related mainland species because island living makes the environment more unpredictable. Since there are limited possibilities to disperse when conditions deteriorate on oceanic islands, individuals are forced to rely on more elaborate behavioral responses to survive. A larger brain supports behavioral flexibility, so birds with larger brains can learn new coping mechanisms rather than evolving specific responses to unpredictable environmental conditions.

โ€œ[O]ur findings fit well with the โ€˜island syndromeโ€™ theory, which predicts that island dwellers have consistent phenotypic differences when compared to mainland populations,โ€ the authors write in their paper.

Unfortunately for the authors of this study, there are plenty of very intelligent birds that do not live on small oceanic islands. Cockatoos, for example (i.e.; more here). Ravens are another example (i.e.; more here). These examples serve to underscore the idea that we still have much to learn about the nature of intelligence and how it evolves.

Source:

Ferran Sayol, Philip A. Downing, Andrew N. Iwaniuk, Joan Maspons, and Daniel Sol (2018). Predictable evolution towards larger brains in birds colonizing oceanic islands, , 9:2820 | doi:10.1038/s41467โ€“018โ€“05280โ€“8

--

--

News and ideas worthy of discourse. Fundamentally informative and intelligently analytical.

Get the Medium app

A button that says 'Download on the App Store', and if clicked it will lead you to the iOS App store
A button that says 'Get it on, Google Play', and if clicked it will lead you to the Google Play store
๐†๐ซ๐ซ๐ฅ๐’๐œ๐ข๐ž๐ง๐ญ๐ข๐ฌ๐ญ, scientist & journalist

PhD evolutionary ecology/ornithology. Psittacophile. scicomm Forbes, previously Guardian. always Ravenclaw. discarded scientist & writer, now an angry house elf